This is a copy of the page I've just put up at
Art Science,
which has some thoughts about how psychology
might be useful in helping people draw.
I'm interested in two things.
First, using psychology and psychological
software to improve my own visual memory, so that I can draw better.
In particular, so that I can sketch people faster and more accurately.
Second, using psychology and psychological software to teach drawing more
efficiently. By analogy with "sports science", I'll call this "art science".
The teaching of drawing has changed little in the past 100 years, and apart
from a bit of pop science about left brain versus right brain, ignores
psychology altogether. But I'm convinced that it has huge amounts to contribute.
By the way, some examples of how it could do so can be seen in the articles
I've linked to about improving musical performance, which have
been written by musicians who are also neuroscientists.
In the rest of this page, I've listed some ideas of
my own, stimulated by things I've read or problems I've
come across while learning to draw. They fall
under these headings: improving visual memory; improving
attention; transcranial
stimulation; blind drawing; mindfulness; divided attention; and
psychological software. I've also added a note at the end about
a psychological phenomenon I've experienced, in case it's not
known.
Improving visual memory
Could the phenomena and methods below help? Are there any others?
- Chunking
This is the way we learn to remember things as entities in their own right
rather than as collections of smaller units. For example, remembering
words as words rather than as sequences of letters.
So could I train myself on, for example, visual patterns often
found in faces, and hence learn to remember faces in terms of these few chunks?
- Flashbulb memory
That is, the way that emotion and interest in an event make memories of it
more vivid and hard to forget than "normal" memories. The deaths of
Kennedy and Diana are famous examples. If I could find a way to arouse
suitable emotions when looking at a subject, would that make
it easier to memorise?
- Tachistoscope training
A tachistoscope displays images to a subject for a very short time,
often for experiments on memory.
There are anecdotes about lab assistants who, after a year or three
preparing such experiments, found that they
were better at remembering the tachistoscope images than they had
been originally. I don't have references, but I seem to remember that
some of these had been experimentally verified. So might
tachistoscope practice help me?
I found a blog posting
"Exercise
V - Tachistoscope - Don't sight-read and chew gum at the same time!", which
describes tachistoscope exercises to improve sight-reading for piano players.
It's from the blog Advanced Sight Reading Piano Music by
Cynthia Irion, an expert pianist and
a neuroscientist and psychologist.
(She writes about many
studies on the psychology of sight-reading. I'd
love to see psychology applied in the same way to drawing.) I also found
an article by
Edward Godnig on
"The
Tachistoscope, its history and uses" which describes
tachistoscope use for training memory and other visual
skills.
- Dreams
I see vivid images in dreams. Some are vivid enough that I could
draw them after I woke up. How can I make myself visualise that
intensely when awake?
- Symbol memory
Although my visual memory is weak, I don't have trouble
learning new alphabets, and even Chinese characters.
Why is there a difference? Can I exploit my symbol-learning
skills to improve my visual memory?
Improving attention
It seems reasonable that if I pay more attention to something,
I'll memorise it better. Artists use several techniques for
improving attention, which I explain under the first two headings.
The third heading refers to some experiments on
a phenomenon which I think might help. Can anything else?
- Negative space
This is a technique in which artists
concentrate on the space between objects rather than the
objects themselves. For example, when drawing spectacles, you look at the
shape not of each lens, but of the space between the face and the frame.
Art teachers justify this by saying that when you look at an object, the
brain is tempted to substitute common and often cartoonish symbols, such as
two circles for a pair of glasses. Looking at the space between objects
stops it doing this, because it has no symbols for that space.
Here are some short articles about it:
"See
Like an Artist: negative space"
by Jay Alders;
"art
stuff - negative space and the left brain" by Kathy Hebert;
and
"Hand
Drawing Demo #6: Reviewing Left Brain/Right Brain"
by Anne Bobroff-Hajal.
- Inverting glasses
I've come across drawing exercises which recommend copying
inverted images. As with negative space, this is
to make the brain concentrate on actual shapes instead of
its symbols for objects. Here are two typical articles about this:
"Upside down Drawing and Contour
Drawing" by "Davy"; and
"Day #031 -
Drawing Picasso’s Igor Stravinsky Upside
Down" by "Neelima".
Inverting spectacles have been used in some
famous
experiments on how subjects adapt to
seeing the world upside down. Could they help artists pay
more attention to shape? Nowadays, one could probably use
Google's computer-in-a-pair-of-specs Google Glass
to run a program that inverts images as you look at them.
- Cold water in the ear
I found the paper "Spatial- and
verbal-memory improvement by cold-water caloric stimulation in healthy subjects",
by D.Bächtold, T.Baumann, P.S.Sándor, M.Kritos, M.Regard, and P.Brugger.
They say that putting cold water in subjects' left ear sped up their
recall of object locations, while water in the right ear sped up recognition of words.
They suggest that the stimulation somehow activates structures in the
opposite hemisphere, speeding up cognitive processes there. Could this be useful?
Transcranial stimulation
Transcranial
direct-current stimulation changes brain activity by applying
weak electric currents to the brain;
transcranial magnetic
stimulation changes activity by inducing currents via
magnetic fields.
As I've said, I want to improve my memory for briefly-seen
images, so that I can better sketch people passing in the street.
There have been a lot of experiments
on using these techniques to enhance cognition: could they help me?
A remark of V.S.Ramachandran in his book Phantoms
in the Brain about the effect of
cold-water stimulation in the ear (I don't have the
book to hand, but it may have been a reference to the study cited
above) made me wonder whether transcranial stimulation
could help artists pay attention to the images of objects, by inhibiting object
recognition. I've already said that artists do so via
negative space, but transcranial stimulation ought to be easier.
It wouldn't need so much practice.
Also, many vision researchers believe that the
brain has some kind of low-level representation of what it sees,
containing amongst other information, that about edges. In other
words, a kind of line drawing. If this is so, is there any
way to give the "drawing" parts of the brain direct access to it?
Drawing something
accurately can be very tricky, needing much estimation and comparison of line
lengths and angles. However, if the brain does keep a reasonably
faithful line drawing of a subject, copying it ought to
be a relatively simple neural process.
Every artist would love to have such an ability.
It has been suggested that this is how the savant artist
Stephen Wiltshire
manages to draw scenes
such as the Houses of Parliament from memory
after only a brief glance at them. So I was excited
when I read that
Allan Snyder in Sydney
claimed to
have enhanced drawing by using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Here's
a quote from New York Times journalist Lawrence Osborne's article
"Savant for a Day":
Two minutes after I started the first drawing [of a cat, under transcranial magnetic
stimulation], I was instructed to try again. After another two minutes, I tried a
third cat, and then in due course a fourth. Then the experiment was over, and the
electrodes were removed. I looked down at my work.
The first felines were boxy and stiffly unconvincing. But
after I had been subjected to about 10 minutes of transcranial magnetic
stimulation, their tails had grown more vibrant,
more nervous; their faces were personable and convincing. They were even
beginning to wear clever expressions.
I could hardly recognize them as my own drawings, though I had watched myself
render each one, in all its loving detail. Somehow over the course of a
very few minutes, and with no additional instruction, I had gone from an
incompetent draftsman to a very impressive artist of the feline form.
I would love to be a subject for such experiments, including on retaining
the ability once stimulation ceases.
I mailed Roi Kadosh at the Cohen
Kadosh Lab in Oxford about these. Unfortunately,
he didn't know anyone with the necessary expertise.
Blind drawing
Blind drawing is drawing without looking at the paper. Some artists use
it to improve hand-eye coordination, or (as with negative space)
to make themselves aware of what's actually seen rather than some symbolic stereotype for it.
The following two articles explain these:
"Blind
Contour Drawing: A Classic Drawing Exercise" by Helen South; and
"Blind
Contour Drawing: Drawing by Touch"
by Carol Rosinski.
I like blind drawing because it produces interesting and sensitive lines,
which have more character than if I draw while looking at the paper.
"Character" is hard to define, but means, amongst other things,
that the line weight and curvature vary more, making the
lines more informative and interesting to look at. The drawings also
have more lines giving information about three-dimensional shape.
For example, the rim of a cheek, as in the woman's face at the
bottom right of the first image on the second row below. Here are some of my blind drawings:
By the way, the first image was somebody on the train to London; the next five are
customers waiting at the counter in the Summertown Costa Coffee; and the last is
competitors in the Wolvercote Midsummer Festival 2013
dog show. The first image was blind except for the right-hand side
of the collar, which I drew blind after glancing at the paper to
position it.
I have two questions. First, what psychological techniques
could make blind drawing more accurate? There is a tale of a caricaturist
who used to attend functions at which he wasn't allowed to draw, and
so would draw surreptitiously on a notepad he carried in his pocket.
Eventually, his drawings became very accurate. Could the
"Templar Cross" software I mention below help? Are there any
methods for teaching the blind to draw which might help?
Second, how can I get the same quality of line in drawings where I'm
looking at the paper as in my blind drawings? Does psychology have
anything to say about this?
A related question is that of copying. Artists often say that their
first rough sketch of a subject is more "alive" than a final copy
made from it. Again, this is because the line in the original has more
character. How can I train myself to make the same kinds of line,
i.e. the same kinds of movement, when copying as when drawing the
original?
Mindfulness
The second row of images shown above, from the Summertown Costa, are from
an exercise where I spent one and a half hours blind-drawing one morning.
It was intensely relaxing, and left
me with an inner glow that lasted for the entire day. I've had similar feelings
before. Once when I was blind drawing from memory in Cornmarket, I felt
an intense buzz as though I'd swallowed a chilli, followed by an intense calm.
Why? Would blind drawing be useful to those who teach mindfulness?
I often feel relaxed after blind drawing, and I'd like to know why that is,
but the episodes above were unusual. Whether or not there's any
correlation with my ability at the time, it was very pleasant, and I'd like to know how to
repeat it. It's better than beer!
Divided attention
In his book A Cure for Gravity, musician Joe Jackson writes about the ability that
good pianists develop to play different music with their left and
right hands:
To keep both hands going, without either playing bum notes all
over the place or losing the steady rhythmic feel, demands
a kind of split consciousness, each hand independent but still
under a centered, Zen-like control.
Is this relevant
to drawing? One needs to
pay attention to both the paper and the subject, but
the paper diverts attention from the subject. What
can psychology tell us about the optimum way to divide attention
between the two, and how to learn to do so?
An analogue to the kind of study I'd like to see is described
in "Hands
Together". This is a posting by neuroanatomist and
piano teacher Tara Gaertner in her blog
Training the Musical Brain: a neuroscience perspective on
teaching and learning music..., in which she discusses
whether it's better to teach piano pieces hands-separately
or hands-together. Amongst the factors she considers are
the effect of dividing attention on learning, the need
for a hand to learn not to mirror the movements of the
opposite hand, and the kinds of practicing behaviours that
learners used.
Psychological software
Here are some suggestions for software that might help
with learning to draw:
- Chunk-display software
By this, I mean software that helps one learn
to chunk the components of common objects. The software
would display an image, and then highlight some
part of it that we want to count as a chunk. It would
then invite me to memorise and draw this part.
Because artists need to pay attention to the relation
between objects, as well as the objects themselves,
some of the chunks might straddle what we'd normally
consider to be two components. For example, software for
helping one chunk the parts of a face might display
the bottom of the nose and the top of the mouth.
- Tachistoscope software
That is, software that implements a
tachistoscope.
- Software for enabling one to see images as line drawings
I want to be able to look at a subject, particularly a
person, and "see" it as a line drawing done to a certain
stylistic convention such as those below from Len Doust's book
Sketching From Life:
Musicians can hear scores as music; can artists
be trained to see subjects as line drawings?
Imagine a program that displays many paired images.
The first image in each pair is a photo. The second is a line drawing
of it made either by an artist or by software such as that in
Mario Costa Sousa and Przemyslaw Prusinkiewicz's paper
"A
few good lines: Suggestive drawing of 3d models".
Could this train my brain to learn associations between parts of the
photo and their line-drawn counterparts, so that I can then mentally
switch from one to the other?
- Blind-drawing the Templar Cross
Here's a Templar Cross:
.
Imagine this painted
on a graphics tablet, or displayed on the screen of a a tablet computer. I
trace it with a stylus, without looking at it. The program works
out where my stylus is, and if it has wandered from the
cross, sounds a tone whose pitch or volume depends on how
far it has wandered. Would this teach me to blind draw
more accurately? One artist I've spoken to thinks it
would, and would be worthwhile.
Visual attraction
This is the phenomenon I mentioned at the start, which
I said I'd describe in case it's not known. I sometimes
practise blind drawing, and drawing from memory,
in the street. A lot of the streets and pavements are
surfaced with asphalt, which has a grey or black
grainy surface
like
this.
When drawing from memory, I try to remember
the subject in detail before looking at the paper.
Sometimes, I find myself doing so with sufficient
intensity that I'm not really aware of my surroundings.
Then I'll suddenly "come to", and find myself
looking at a pattern in the asphalt which looks similar to
what I was thinking about. It feels bizarre, as
though I've created the pattern by force of intense
concentration. But presumably what's happened is that
I've unconsciously directed my gaze towards some region
of the asphalt that looks similar to what I was thinking of,
and I've only then become aware of it.